進階搜尋


   電子論文尚未授權公開,紙本請查館藏目錄
(※如查詢不到或館藏狀況顯示「閉架不公開」,表示該本論文不在書庫,無法取用。)
系統識別號 U0026-1508201219295400
論文名稱(中文) 老年人抑制干擾能力與補償反應的探討
論文名稱(英文) The Investigation of Inhibiting Interference Function and Compensatory Responses in Elderly
校院名稱 成功大學
系所名稱(中) 心理學系認知科學碩士班
系所名稱(英) MS in Cognitive Science
學年度 100
學期 2
出版年 101
研究生(中文) 方偉翰
研究生(英文) Wei-Han Fang
學號 U76991057
學位類別 碩士
語文別 中文
論文頁數 79頁
口試委員 指導教授-謝淑蘭
口試委員-蔡玲玲
口試委員-廖敏如
中文關鍵字 老化  旁側夾擊作業  補償反應  事件相關腦電位波  側邊準備腦電位波 
英文關鍵字 Aging  Flanker task  Compensatory responses  Event-related potential  Lateralized readiness potential 
學科別分類
中文摘要 本研究欲探討年輕人與老年人的旁側夾擊效應是否有差異,並觀察老年人是否會啟動補償反應以克服抑制方面可能的缺損;也探討老年人的補償反應是否會隨著作業要求的程度不同而改變。為達目標,本研究於三個旁側夾擊派典實驗間操弄兩種「刺激-反應」配置情況:「刺激-反應」相容(PRO)與「刺激-反應」不相容(ANTI)情況,並利用此兩種不同情況的嘗試次比例的多寡來調整作業要求的程度。年輕人與老年人於旁側夾擊的作業中,要針對中間的目標物做反應,並抑制兩旁干擾物方向的影響,無論其方向性是與目標物一致、不一致或無干擾的中性情況。本研究除了利用行為測量外,也量測多個事件相關腦電位波(event-related potentials, ERPs)與側邊準備腦電位波(lateralized readiness potential, LRP)來系統性觀察老年人是否能產生補償反應以抵抗旁側物帶來的干擾。結果發現三個實驗間老年人反應時間的旁側夾擊效應無增加趨勢,老年人整體正確率較高,但未隨著作業要求程度的改變觀察到正確率旁側夾擊效應改變的趨勢。此外由ERPs與LRP的指標我們也發現老年人整體運用了一些補償反應以彌補較缺損的抑制干擾能力。雖然這些補償反應並未隨著作業要求程度的變換而改變,不過藉由腦電位波的變化我們發現老年人會傾向投注較多視覺注意力於目標物上,且採用較保守小心的策略與高反應閾值於抑制干擾物上。透過這些策略與補償反應的運用,老年人是可以如同年輕人般有效的抑制干擾物的影響。
英文摘要 The present study investigated whether there is an age-related flanker effect, whether older adults employ compensatory responses to overcome flanker interferences, and whether older adults’ compensatory responses may vary as a function of the degree of task demands. To achieve these goals, we manipulated different degrees of task demands in a flanker-task paradigm, by arranging different proportion of two stimulus-response mapping rules that includes compatible (PRO) or incompatible (ANTI) responses. The young and older adults focused the directions on centrally presented target arrows and inhibiting to the opposite direction (incongruent), the same direction (congruent), or neutral flankers. Behavioral data, event-related potentials (ERPs), and lateralized readiness potential (LRP) were measured to observe whether the older adults resist flanker interference. Throughout the three experiments of different degrees of task demands, we found the similar amount of flanker effects in reaction time between young and older adults. The older adults showed the greater overall accuracy and decreased flanker effects on accuracy across three experiments compared to the young adults. The older adults engaged some compensatory responses were comfirmed by the ERP and LRP components. Although these compensatory responses in older adults did not vary as a function of different degrees of task demands, the results suggest the older adults recruited a top-down strategy of increased attention to the target arrow, and adopt a higher response threshold to maintain the better performance. Through the engagement of compensatory responses, the older adults could overcome the flanker interference compared to young adults.
論文目次 第一章 緒論 1
1.1 認知老化研究簡介 1
1.2 抑制功能與作業派典介紹 4
1.3 抑制功能與老化策略控制的進一步操弄 7
1.3.1 補償反應的引發及學習介入 8
1.3.2 知覺層面的操弄 8
1.3.3 比例的操弄 9
1.3.4 動作與知覺層面的抑制 11
1.4 本研究派典 12
1.5 研究動機與預期結果 16
第二章 研究方法 18
2.0 前言 18
2.1 受試者 18
2.2 實驗設備與刺激 21
2.3 實驗流程與設計 22
2.4 電生理記錄 23
2.5 資料分析 24
2.5.1 行為資料(正確率、反應時間) 24
2.5.2 腦波資料 24
2.5.2.1 刺激鎖定腦電位波: N1, N2和P3 25
2.5.2.2 反應鎖定腦電位波: ERN和CRN 25
2.5.2.3 刺激鎖定側邊準備腦電位波 25
第三章 研究結果 27
3.1 行為資料 27
3.1.1 正確率 27
3.1.2 反應時間 29
3.2 腦波資料 31
3.2.1 刺激鎖定腦電位波: N1, N2, P3 31
3.2.1.1 N1峰值振幅 31
3.2.1.2 N2平均振幅 33
3.2.1.3 P3峰值潛時 36
3.2.2 反應鎖定腦電位波: ERN, CRN 39
3.2.2.1 ERN峰間幅值 39
3.2.2.2 CRN峰間幅值 42
3.2.3 刺激鎖定側邊準備腦電位波 44
3.2.3.1 LRP於不相容情況下不一致情況負向反折平均振幅 45
3.2.3.2 LRP正確方向波峰振幅 46
3.2.3.3 LRP 20%正確方向潛時 47
第四章 總結與討論 51
4.1 行為資料(正確率與反應時間) 52
4.2 腦波資料 54
4.2.1 N1 54
4.2.2 N2 55
4.2.3 P3 57
4.2.4 ERN 58
4.2.5 CRN 59
4.2.6 LRP 60
4.3 研究限制與混淆變項 61
4.4 結論 63
參考文獻 65
附錄 73
參考文獻 Bartholow, B. D., Pearson, M. A., Dickter, C. L., Sher, K. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2005). Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: beyond errors and response conflict. Psychophysiology, 42(1), 33-42. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00258.x
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. F. (1996). Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories-IA and-II in Psychiatric Outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67(3), 588-597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13
Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44(1), 195-208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006
Cerella, J. (1985). Age-Related Decline in Extrafoveal Letter Perception. Journal of Gerontology, 40(6), 727-736. doi: 10.1093/geronj/40.6.727
Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., & Scalf, P. (2005). The implications of cortical recruitment and brain morphology for individual differences in inhibitory function in aging humans. Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 363-375. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.363
Coles, M. G. H. (1989). Modern Mind-Brain Reading: Psychophysiology, Physiology, and Cognition. Psychophysiology, 26(3), 251-269. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1989.tb01916.x
Coles, M. G. H., Scheffers, M. K., & Holroyd, C. B. (2001). Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing. Biological Psychology, 56(3), 173-189. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(01)00076-x
Cook, I. A., Bookheimer, S. Y., Mickes, L., Leuchter, A. F., & Kumar, A. (2007). Aging and brain activation with working memory tasks: an fMRI study of connectivity. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(4), 332-342. doi: 10.1002/gps.1678
Dempster, F., & Corkill, A. (1999). Interference and Inhibition in Cognition and Behavior: Unifying Themes for Educational Psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 11(1), 1-88. doi: 10.1023/a:1021992632168
Di Russo, F., Martínez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2003). Source Analysis of Event-related Cortical Activity during Visuo-spatial Attention. Cerebral Cortex, 13(5), 486-499. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.5.486
Doucet, C., & Stelmack, R. M. (1999). The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time. Psychophysiology, 36(3), 351-363.
Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 65(1), 49-59. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.49
Endrass, T., Schreiber, M., & Kathmann, N. (2012). Speeding up older adults: age-effects on error processing in speed and accuracy conditions. Biological Psychology, 89(2), 426-432. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.005
Eppinger, B., Kray, J., Mecklinger, A., & John, O. (2007). Age differences in task switching and response monitoring: evidence from ERPs. Biological Psychology, 75(1), 52-67. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.12.001
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 78(6), 447-455. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial. Biological Psychology, 51(2–3), 87-107. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00031-9
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (2001). Changes of error-related ERPs with age. Experimental Brain Research, 138(2), 258-262. doi: 10.1007/s002210100712
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340-347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886
Fernandez-Duque, D., & Black, S. E. (2006). Attentional networks in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 133-143. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.133
Fernandez-Duque, D., & Black, S. E. (2008). Selective attention in early Dementia of Alzheimer Type. Brain Cogn, 66(3), 221-231. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.08.003
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
Ford, J. M. (1999). Schizophrenia: The broken P300 and beyond. Psychophysiology, 36(06), 667-682. doi: null
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101-135. doi: Doi 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101
Gamboz, N., Zamarian, S., & Cavallero, C. (2010). Age-Related Differences in the Attention Network Test (ANT). Experimental Aging Research, 36(3), 287-305. doi: 10.1080/0361073x.2010.484729
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385-390. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x
Gehring, W. J., Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Probability effects on stimulus evaluation and response processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 198-216. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.1.198
Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An Extension of Box's Results on the Use of the F Distribution in Multivariate Analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29(3), 885-891.
Germain, S., & Collette, F.. (2008). Dissociation of perceptual and motor inhibitory processes in young and elderly participants using the Simon task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(06), 1014-1021. doi: doi:10.1017/S135561770808123X
Grady, C. L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Changes in memory processing with age. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10(2), 224-231. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00073-8
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1988). Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: A psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 331-344. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.583 10.1037/0096-1523.14.3.331
Guerreiro, M. J., Murphy, D. R., & Van Gerven, P. W. (2010). The role of sensory modality in age-related distraction: a critical review and a renewed view. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 975-1022. doi: 10.1037/a0020731
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View. In H. Bower Gordon (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. Volume 22, pp. 193-225): Academic Press.
Hedden, T., Van Dijk, K. R., Shire, E. H., Sperling, R. A., Johnson, K. A., & Buckner, R. L. (2012). Failure to modulate attentional control in advanced aging linked to white matter pathology. Cereb Cortex, 22(5), 1038-1051. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr172
Hoffmann, S., & Falkenstein, M. (2011). Aging and Error Processing: Age Related Increase in the Variability of the Error-Negativity Is Not Accompanied by Increase in Response Variability. PLoS One, 6(2), e17482. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017482
Hsieh, S., Liang, Y. C., & Tsai, Y. C. (2012). Do age-related changes contribute to the flanker effect? Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(5), 960-972. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.013
Jennings, J. M., Dagenbach, D., E., Christine M., & Funke, L. J. (2007). Age-Related Changes and the Attention Network Task: An Examination of Alerting, Orienting, and Executive Function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(4), 353-369. doi: 10.1080/13825580600788837
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 38(3), 557-577. doi: 10.1017/s0048577201990559
Kopp, B., Mattler, U., & Rist, F. (1994). Selective attention and response competition in schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Research, 53(2), 129-139. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90104-x
Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33(3), 282-294. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x
Kramer, A. F., Humphrey, D. G., Larish, J. F., & Logan, G. D. (1994). Aging and inhibition: Beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. Psychology and Aging, 9(4), 491-512. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.4.491
Kutas, M, McCarthy, G, & Donchin, E. (1977). Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science, 197(4305), 792-795. doi: 10.1126/science.887923
Kutas, M., & Donchin, E. (1980). Preparation to Respond as Manifested by Movement-Related Brain Potentials. Brain Research, 202(1), 95-115. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(80)80037-0
Leuthold, H., & Jentzsch, I. (2002). Distinguishing neural sources of movement preparation and execution: An electrophysiological analysis. Biological Psychology, 60(2–3), 173-198. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00032-7
Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 12(3), 410-432. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611
Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mouloua, M., Woldorff, M. G., Clark, V. P., & Hawkins, H. L. (1994). Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 887-904. doi: 10.1038/363345a0
Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. (2007). Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a "new view". In Gorfein D.S. & MacLeod C.M. (Eds.), The Place of Inhibition in Cognition (pp. 145-162). Washington: American Psychological Association.
Luu, P., Collins, P., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: Negative affect and emotionality in relation to frontal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 43-60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00438.x
Machado, L., Devine, A., & Wyatt, N. (2009). Distractibility with advancing age and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 47(7), 1756-1764. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.018
Madden, D. J., & Gottlob, L. R. (1997). Adult age differences in strategic and dynamic components of focusing visual attention. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 4(3), 185-210. doi: 10.1080/13825589708256647
Mathewson, K. J., Dywan, J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2005). Brain bases of error-related ERPs as influenced by age and task. Biological Psychology, 70(2), 88-104. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.12.005
Maylor, E. A., Birak, K. S., & Schlaghecken, F. (2011). Inhibitory motor control in old age: evidence for de-automatization? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 132. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00132
Miller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology, 35(1), 99-115. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3510099
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Nassauer, K. W., & Halperin, J. M. (2003). Dissociation of perceptual and motor inhibition processes through the use of novel computerized conflict tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(01), 25-30. doi: doi:10.1017/S1355617703910034
Nessler, D., Friedman, D., Johnson Jr, R., & Bersick, M. (2007). ERPs suggest that age affects cognitive control but not response conflict detection. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(11), 1769-1782. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.07.011
Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K., Talsma, D., Coles, M. G. H., Holroyd, C., Kok, A., & van der Molen, M. (2002). A computational account of altered error processing in older age: Dopamine and the error-related negativity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 19-36. doi: 10.3758/cabn.2.1.19
Osman, A., & Moore, C. M. (1993). The locus of dual-task interference: Psychological refractory effects on movement-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(6), 1292-1312. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.5.965.1994-08227-00110.1037/0096-1523.19.5.965 10.1037/0096-1523.19.6.1292
Phillips, L. H., & Andres, P. (2010). The cognitive neuroscience of aging: New findings on compensation and connectivity. Cortex, 46(4), 421-424. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.01.005
Pietschmann, M., Endrass, T., & Kathmann, N. (2011). Age-related alterations in performance monitoring during and after learning. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(7), 1320-1330. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.07.016
Pietschmann, M., Simon, K., Endrass, T., & Kathmann, N. (2008). Changes of performance monitoring with learning in older and younger adults. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 559-568. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00651.x
Polich, J. (1997). EEG and ERP assessment of normal aging. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 104(3), 244-256. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(97)96139-6
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231
Purmann, S., Badde, S., Luna-Rodriguez, A., & Wendt, M. (2011). Adaptation to frequent conflict in the Eriksen Flanker Task: An ERP study. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25(2), 50-59. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000041
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(9), 394-400. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01957-5
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306(5695), 443-447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301
Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Robertson, E. R., Mikels, J. A., Carstensen, L. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Selective attention to emotion in the aging brain. Psychology and Aging, 24(3), 519-529. doi: 10.1037/a0016952
Schaie, K. W. (2004). Cognitive aging. In Richard W. Pew & Susan B. Van Hemel (Eds.), Technology for Adaptive Aging (pp. 41-63). Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Schaie, K. W., Willis, S. L., & Caskie, G. I. (2004). The Seattle longitudinal study: relationship between personality and cognition. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn, 11(2-3), 304-324. doi: 10.1080/13825580490511134
Schreiber, M., Pietschmann, M., Kathmann, N., & Endrass, T. (2011). ERP correlates of performance monitoring in elderly. Brain Cognition, 76(1), 131-139. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.02.003
Shaw, R. J. (1991). Age-related increases in the effects of automatic semantic activation. Psychology and Aging, 6(4), 595-604. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.6.4.595
Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2001). Using the jackknife-based scoring method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs. Psychophysiology, 38(5), 816-827. doi: doi:null
Van 't Ent, D. (2002). Perceptual and motor contributions to performance and ERP components after incorrect motor activation in a flanker reaction task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113(2), 270-283. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00730-1
Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 593-602. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045837
Verbruggen, F., Liefooghe, B., Notebaert, W., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005). Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. Acta Psychol (Amst), 120(3), 307-326. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.05.003
Verhaeghen, F., & Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 849-857. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00071-4
Verhaeghen, F., & De Meersman, L. (1998). Aging and the Stroop effect: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), 120-126. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.120
Vidal, F., Hasbroucq, T., Grapperon, J., & Bonnet, M. (2000). Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? Biological Psychology, 51(2–3), 109-128. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00032-0
West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 272-292. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272
Wild-Wall, N., Falkenstein, M., & Hohnsbein, J. (2008). Flanker interference in young and older participants as reflected in event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1211(0), 72-84. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.025
Willemssen, R., Hoormann, J., Hohnsbein, J., & Falkenstein, M. (2004). Central and parietal event-related lateralizations in a flanker task. Psychophysiology, 41(5), 762-771. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00201.x
Wright, L. L., & Elias, J. W. (1979). Age Differences in the Effects of Perceptual Noise. Journal of Gerontology, 34(5), 704-708. doi: 10.1093/geronj/34.5.704
Yordanova, J., Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., & Kolev, V. (2004). Parallel systems of error processing in the brain. NeuroImage, 22(2), 590-602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.040
Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Hohnsbein, J., & Falkenstein, M. (2004). Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials. Brain, 127(Pt 2), 351-362. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh042
Zeef, E. J., & Kok, A. (1993). Age-related differences in the timing of stimulus and response processes during visual selective attention: performance and psychophysiological analyses. Psychophysiology, 30(2), 138-151.
Zeef, E. J., Sonke, C. J., Kok, A., Buiten, M. M., & Kenemans, J. L. (1996). Perceptual factors affecting age-related differences in focused attention: performance and psychophysiological analyses. Psychophysiology, 33(5), 555-565. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02432.x
Zhu, D. C., Zacks, R. T., & Slade, J. M. (2010). Brain activation during interference resolution in young and older adults: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 50(2), 810-817. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.087
論文全文使用權限
  • 同意授權校內瀏覽/列印電子全文服務,於2013-08-27起公開。


  • 如您有疑問,請聯絡圖書館
    聯絡電話:(06)2757575#65773
    聯絡E-mail:etds@email.ncku.edu.tw