進階搜尋


   電子論文尚未授權公開,紙本請查館藏目錄
(※如查詢不到或館藏狀況顯示「閉架不公開」,表示該本論文不在書庫,無法取用。)
系統識別號 U0026-0802201600255100
論文名稱(中文) 自閉症及亞斯伯格症孩童社交互動表現的認知因子之探討
論文名稱(英文) Identifying cognitive correlates of social interactions in children with Autistic disorder and Asperger syndrome
校院名稱 成功大學
系所名稱(中) 職能治療學系
系所名稱(英) Department of Occupational Therapy
學年度 104
學期 1
出版年 105
研究生(中文) 賴思慎
研究生(英文) Szu-Shen Lai
學號 T76021021
學位類別 碩士
語文別 英文
論文頁數 93頁
口試委員 指導教授-陳官琳
口試委員-鳳華
口試委員-吳進欽
口試委員-蔡景宏
中文關鍵字 自閉症類群障礙症  交互行為  社交互動 
英文關鍵字 autism spectrum disorder  reciprocity  social interaction 
學科別分類
中文摘要 社交互動(social interaction)缺損是自閉症類群障礙症(autism spectrum disorder, ASD)孩童的核心特徵。ASD孩童社交互動當中亦缺乏適當的交互行為(reciprocity)。交互行為是產生合作行為的重要過程,讓人與人間可以一起進行活動並達成共同的目標。兒童社交互動及交互行為的發展仰賴日漸成熟的認知及情緒功能。過去研究指出與交互行為相關的腦部區域為控制心智理論(theory of mind)和執行功能(executive function)的腦區。但迄今未有行為研究同時考量二者探討ASD孩童交互行為。再加上在實驗室情境中,以結構化評估工具無法反應孩童交互行為缺損。若使用和日常生活的情境較相似的非結構化評估工具,可能可較精確評估與日常生活中較相似的交互行為。此外,目前探討ASD孩童社交互動的研究,大多僅使用心智理論和冷執行功能來解釋ASD孩童的社交缺損,未考量熱執行功能,即忽略情緒及動機對於社交互動重要性。
因此,為解決上述文獻的限制,本研究有三目的分別為探討ASD孩童(一)冷執行功能、熱執行功能、心智理論三個因子對交互行為之相關性;(二)冷執行功能、熱執行功能、心智理論三個因子與社交互動之相關性;(三)實驗室情境中交互行為與日常生活情境中社交互動之相關性。
本研究從台南、高雄、台北的醫院及診所招募59位4至12歲依據精神疾病診斷與統計手冊第四版(The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders- fourth edition, DSM-IV)的自閉症(autistic disorder)及亞斯伯格症(Asperger’s syndrome)孩童。分別以心智理論測驗(Theory of Mind Task battery)、孩童賭博作業(Children’s gambling task)、卡片向度改變分類作業(Dimensional card change sort task , DCCS)、互動式繪畫作業(Interactive Drawing Task , IDT)、社交反應量表學齡版(Social Responsiveness Scale-second edition school aged form, SRS-2)、自閉症兒童行為檢核表(Childhood Autism Rating Scale, CARS)、魏氏兒童智力量表第四版/魏氏幼兒智力量表第四版(Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth Edition, WISC-IV/Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Fourth Edition, WPPSI - IV)、文蘭適應行為量表(Vinland Adaptive Behavior Scale , VABS)評估兒童的心智理論、冷執行功能、熱執行功能、交互行為、自閉症症狀嚴重程度、語言能力、日常生活中的社交互動。統計分析包含(一)以描述性統計分析孩童的基本資料;(二)使用皮爾森相關係數(Pearson’s correlations coefficient)分析冷執行功能、熱執行功能、心智理論三個因子與交互行為及社交互動的相關性;(三)以冷執行功能、熱執行功能、心智理論為獨變項,使用逐步線性迴歸(stepwise linear regression analysis)分析三因子對於交互行為與社交互動之影響。
研究結果顯示自閉症及亞斯伯格症的孩童(一)冷執行功能中的單向度正確率(r =.482, p <.01 )、雙向度正確率(r =.501, p <.01)及熱執行功能(r =.396, p <.05)與交互行為有顯著的相關;在迴歸分析中,雙向度正確率與熱執行功能為顯著的預測因子,對交互行為有31.2%的解釋性;(二) 冷執行功能中的雙向度正確率(r =.348, p <.01)、單向度正確率(r = -.339, p <.01)、雙向度反應時間(r =-.296, p <.05)、轉換時間差(r =.382, p <.01) 與心智理論(r =.471, p <.01)與社交互動有顯著的相關性;在迴歸分析中,語言能力、症狀嚴重程度、執行功能中的轉換時間差為顯著的預測因子,對社交互動有47%的解釋性;(三)實驗室情境中交互行為(r =.290, p <.05)與日常生活情境中社交互動有顯著的相關。
本研究對於自閉症及亞斯伯格症之孩童的心智理論能力、冷執行功能、熱執行功能、交互行為與社交互動的相關性提供進一步的瞭解。研究結果顯示若自閉症及亞斯伯格症孩童有較佳的冷執行功能與熱執行功能可能會有較佳的交互行為、有較佳的心智理論與執行功能可能會有較佳的社交互動表現,以及有較佳的交互行為能力則可能會有較好的社交互動表現。然而,本研究有四限制,包含(一) 結果無法類化至低功能且無口語的ASD及未分類廣泛性發展障礙(pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified, PDD-NOS)診斷之孩童;(二) 心智理論能力可能因為測驗需要較好的語言能力而被低估;(三) 本研究並未考量孩童的交互行為可能受到社交互動風格(social interaction style)影響; (四) 本研究VABS教室版由家長填答,但應由老師填答。因此,建議未來研究可招募低功能且無口語的ASD、未分類廣泛性發展障礙診斷之孩童、使用較不需口語的心智理論評估工具、增加評估社交互動風格的工具、採納老師填答之學校社交互動表現,以更進一步闡明社交互動中所需的認知相關因子。此研究的結果協助我們找出影響自閉症及亞斯伯格症孩童日常生活社交互動及交互行為的認知因子,進而作為臨床依據改善自閉症及亞斯伯格症孩童的社交缺損,並進一步提升其日常生活的社交參與。
英文摘要 Social interaction deficits are core symptoms of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Reciprocity deficit, one of the social interaction deficits in children with ASD, is the process of collaborative behavior between individuals who perform activities to reach shared goals. The reciprocity and social interaction deficits of children with ASD lead to difficulties in social participation in real-life contexts. Reciprocal behaviors and social interactions depend on the child’s developing cognitive skills and emotional functioning. In previous studies, the brain regions controlling theory of mind (ToM) and executive function (EF) were found to be the neural correlates of reciprocity. However, to date, no behavioral studies have investigated the relations among ToM, EF, and reciprocity in children with ASD. Moreover, the structured computer games used in past studies could not reflect the performance of children with ASD in real life contexts. In addition, ToM and cool EF have been examined to explain the social interaction deficit in children with ASD. However, based on current evidence these two abilities cannot fully explain the social deficits in children with ASD, possibly because the influences of motivation- and emotion-related factors, such as hot EF, were not examined in past studies
To resolve the above gaps, this study aimed to investigate in children with autistic disorder and Asperger’s syndrome (1) the correlates of reciprocity by considering cool EF, hot EF, and ToM; (2) the correlates of social interactions in daily life by considering cool EF, hot EF, and ToM; and (3) the correlation between reciprocity in a laboratory context and social interactions in a real-life context by using a relatively unstructured measure to assess reciprocity.
To fulfill these aims, 59 children with diagnoses of autistic disorder or Asperger’s syndrome according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-fourth edition (DSM-IV) and aged from 4 to 12 years were recruited from clinics and hospitals in Tainan, Kaohsiung, and Taipei. Their ToM, hot and cool EF, reciprocity, social interactions, autistic symptom severity, and verbal abilities were examined with the Theory of Mind Task Battery (TOMB), Dimensional Card Change Sort Task (DCCS), Children’s Gambling Task, Interactive Drawing Task (IDT), Vinland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS), Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS), and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) or Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV). For the data analysis, (1) descriptive analysis was used to analyze the demographic data of the children; (2) Pearson’s correlation coefficients were used to analyze the correlations among cool EF, hot EF, and ToM to reciprocity and social interaction, respectively; and (3) stepwise regression and hierarchical regression models with a stepwise method were used to examine the predictors of reciprocity in the laboratory context and social interactions in real-life contexts, respectively.
The results of the present study showed the following: (1) Cool EF-single block accuracy (r =.482, p <.01), mixed block accuracy (r =.501, p <.01), and hot EF (r =.396, p <.05) were significantly correlated with reciprocity; in the stepwise regression and hierarchical regression models with a stepwise method, cool EF-mixed block accuracy and hot EF were significant predictors, jointly explaining 31.2% of the variance in reciprocity. (2) Cool EF-mixed block accuracy (r =.348, p <.01), single block reaction time (r = -.339, p <.01), mixed block reaction time (r =-.296, p <.05), shifting cost (r =.382, p <.01), and ToM (r =.471, p <.01) were significantly correlated with social interaction; in the stepwise regression and hierarchical regression models with a stepwise method, verbal ability, autistic symptom severity, and cool EF-shifting cost were the significant predictors, jointly explaining 47% of the variance in social interaction. (3) Reciprocity in the laboratory context (r =.290, p <.05) was significantly correlated with social interaction in daily life.
The present study provides further understanding of the relationships among ToM, cool EF, hot EF, reciprocity, and social interaction. The results indicate that children with autistic disorder and Asperger’s syndrome who have better cool and hot EF capacity tend to have better reciprocity capacity, that those who have better ToM and cool EF ability tend to have better social interaction performance, and that better reciprocity capacity has a positive correlation with social interaction. However, the limitations of the present study include the following: (1) The findings cannot be generalized to lower-functioning and non-verbal ASD and children diagnosed with PDD-NOS; (2) ToM ability may have been underestimated due to the verbal demands of the ToM tasks ; (3) social interaction style was not examined, and (4) the VABS-classroom version was reported by caregivers. Therefore, further study is warranted to (1) recruit lower-functioning children with less verbal ability and children diagnosed with PDD-NOS; (2) apply a ToM measurement which is less verbally demanding; (3) add a measure for assessing social interaction styles, and (4) collect VABS-classroom version reports from teachers to better illustrate the cognitive correlates in social interactions. The results of the present study should help clinicians to identify the cognitive correlates of reciprocity and social interactions in children with autistic disorder and Asperger’s syndrome, which can be the basis of clinical interventions for reducing children’s social interactions deficits and improving their social participations in daily life. In addition, the results may also serve as a foundation for further studies to identify the correlates of social interactions in children with ASD.
論文目次  TABLE OF CONTENTS
中文摘要 I
ABSTRACT IV
致謝 VIII
TABLE OF CONTENTS X
LIST OF TABLES XIV
LIST OF FIGURES XV
LIST OF APPENDICES XVI
CHAPTER 1 Introduction 1
CHAPTER 2 Literature Review 6
2.1. Autism Spectrum Disorder 6
2.1.1. Diagnosis 6
2.1.2. Prevalence 9
2.1.3. Prognosis 10
2.2. Social interaction in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 10
2.3. Reciprocity 11
2.3.1. Definition and importance of reciprocity 11
2.3.2. Neural correlates of reciprocity 12
2.3.3. Development of reciprocity 12
2.3.4. Reciprocity in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 13
2.4. Theory of mind 14
2.4.1. Definition and importance 14
2.4.2. Development of theory of mind 15
2.4.3. Theory of mind in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 16
2.5. Executive Function 17
2.5.1. Definition and importance 17
2.5.2. Cool executive function and hot executive function and their respective importance 17
2.5.3. Cool executive function in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 19
2.5.4. Hot executive function in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 20
2.6. The relationships between theory of mind and reciprocity in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 21
2.7. The relationships between executive function and reciprocity in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 21
2.8. The relationships between theory of mind and social interactions in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 22
2.9. The relationships between executive function and social interactions in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 22
2.10. Summary of the literature review 23
2.11. Schematic representation of human reciprocity in social interactions 24
CHAPTER 3 Methods 26
3.1. Purposes and hypotheses 26
3.2. Participants 26
3.3. Measures 27
3.3.1. Interactive Drawing Test (IDT) 28
3.3.2. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS) 29
3.3.3. Dimensional Card Change Sort Task (DCCS) 30
3.3.4. Children’s Gambling Task 32
3.3.5. Theory of Mind Task Battery 33
3.3.6. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children® - Fourth Edition (WISC®-IV) or Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence™ - Fourth Edition (WPPSI™ - IV) 34
3.3.7. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) 35
3.3.8. Child Autism Rating Scale 36
3.4. Procedures 37
3.4.1. Measurement preparation 37
3.4.2. Study procedure 38
3.5. Data analysis 39
CHAPTER 4 Results 41
4.1. Demographic characteristics of the participants 41
4.2. Descriptive analysis of ToM, cool EF, hot EF, reciprocity and social interaction 42
4.3. The relationships among ToM, cool EF, hot EF, reciprocity and social interaction 43
4.4. The predictors of reciprocity 44
4.5. The predictors of social interaction 45
CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION 46
5.1. The relationships between ToM, cool EF, hot EF and reciprocity 46
5.2. The relationships between ToM, cool EF, hot EF and social interaction 50
5.3. The relationship between reciprocity and social interaction 53
5.4. Limitations 54
5.5. Contributions and Implications 55
5.6. Conclusions 56
References 58

LIST OF TABLES
Table 1. Outcomes of each card in the advantageous and disadvantageous decks. 72
Table 2. Demographic characteristics of the participants 73
Table 3. Descriptive analysis of ToM, cool EF, hot EF, reciprocity and social
interaction 74
Table 4. Correlations on measurements of interest in children with ASD 75
Table 5. Stepwise linear regression analysis of reciprocity in children with ASD (N=59) 76
Table 6. Hierarchical regression analysis with stepwise method of reciprocity in children with ASD (N=59) 77
Table 7. Stepwise linear regression analysis of social interaction in children with ASD (N=59) 78
Table 8. Hierarchical regression analysis with stepwise method of social interaction in children with ASD (N=59) 79


LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1. Schematic representation of human reciprocity in social interactions 80
Figure 2. Measurements corresponding to the measured abilities 81
Figure 3. Schematic illustration of two sample trials from a single task block of the
Dimensional Change Card Sort Task 82
Figure 4. Sample card face from the disadvantageous deck (Gain= 2, Loss= -4) 83
Figure 5. Procedure of the study 84
Figure 6. Overview of the number of participants for recruitment and the reason for exclusion 85








LIST OF APPENDICES
Appendix A. Diagnostic criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder in DSM-5 86
Appendix B. Diagnostic criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder in DSM-IV-TR 88
Appendix C. The inputs of IDT 92
Appendix D. The human study appoval 93

參考文獻 American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and stastistical manual of mental dosorders (4 ed). Washington,DC: American Psychiatric Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and stastistical manual of mental dosorders (5 ed). Washington,DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11(2), 231-239.
Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (2005). Introduction: why language matters. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.),. Why language matters for theory of mind, 3-25.
Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical explanations of children's understanding of the mind. . British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(1), 7-31.
Backer van Ommeren, T., Koot, H. M., Scheeren, A. M., & Begeer, S. (2015). Reliability and validity of the Interactive Drawing Test: A measure of reciprocity for children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(7), 1967-1977. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2353-x
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(2), 285-297.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Do people with autism understand what causes emotion? Child Development, 62(2), 385-395.
Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813-822.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46.
Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cognition, 55(1), 30-40. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1-3), 7-15.
Begeer, S., Malle, B. F., Nieuwland, M., & Keysar, B. (2010). Using theory of mind to represent and take part in social interactions: comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and typically developing controls. European Journal of Child Development, 7, 104-122.
Billstedt, E., Gillberg, I. C., & Gillberg, C. (2011). Aspects of quality of life in adults diagnosed with autism in childhood: a population-based study. Autism, 15(1), 7-20. doi: 10.1177/1362361309346066
Blumberg, S. J., Bramlett, M. D., Kogan, M. D., Schieve, L. A., Jones, J. R., & Lu, M. C. (2013). Changes in prevalence of parent-reported autism spectrum disorder in school-aged U.S. children: 2007 to 2011-2012. National Health Stastistics Reports(65), 1-11.
Breidboard, J. & Croudace, T. J. (2013). Reliability generalization for Childhood Autism Rating Scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, (12): 2855-65. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1832-9.
Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delays. Autism, 2, 325-344.
Carpenter, M. (2009). Just how joint is joint action in infancy? Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 380-392.
Castelloe, P., & Dawson, G. (1993). Subclassification of children with autism and pervasive developmental disorder: a questionnaire based on Wing's subgrouping scheme. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(2), 229-241.
Centers for Diease Control and Prevention. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and mortality weekly report. Surveillance summaries (Washington, D.C. : 2002), 63(2), 1-22
Chamberlain, B., Kasari, C., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2007). Involvement or isolation? The social networks of children with autism in regular classrooms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 230-242. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0164-4
Chang, C. L., Lung, F. W., Yen, C. F., & Yang, P. (2013). Adaptive behaviors in high-functioning Taiwanese children with autism spectrum disorders: an investigation of the mediating roles of symptoms severity and cognitive ability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1347-1355. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1684-8
Charman, T., & Baird, G. (2002). Practitioner review: diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(3), 289-305.
Chen, H. Y., Chen, Y. H., Keith, T. Z. & Chang, B. S. (2009). What does the WISC-IV measure? validation of the scoring and CHC-based interpretative approaches. Journal of Research in Education Sciences (54 )3, 85-108
Chen, Y. H., & Chen, H. Y. (2007). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-Fourth Edition: Chinese version manual (Wechsler, D.). Taipei, Taiwan: The Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation (Original work published 2003)
Chen, Y. H., & Chen, H. Y. (2013). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Fourth Edition: Chinese version manual (Wechsler, D.). Taipei, Taiwan: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation (Original work published 2012)
Cohen, H. D., Rosen, R. C., & Goldstein, L. (1976). Electroencephalographic laterality changes during human sexual orgasm. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 5(3), 189-199.
Cole, T., & Teboul, J. C. B. (2004). Non-zero-sum collaboration, reciprocity, and the preference for similarity: developing an adaptive model of close relational functioning. Personal Relationships, 11, 135-160.
Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., & Reich, W. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427-433.
Constantino, J. N., Hudziak, J. J., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Deficits in reciprocal social behavior in male twins: evidence for a genetically independent domain of psychopathology. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolesc Psychiatry, 42(4), 458-467. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000046811.95464.21
Dahlgren, S. O., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). Theory of mind in non-retarded children with autism and Asperger's syndrome. A research note. Jouranal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(6), 759-763.
Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3(1), 11-21.
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Kalin, N. H. (2000). Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 126(6), 890-909.
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479-485.
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 568-570.
Diamond, A., & Kirkham, N. (2005). Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16(4), 291-297. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01530.x
Dichter, G. S., Radonovich, K. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Lam, K. S., Holtzclaw, T. N., & Bodfish, J. W. (2010). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the dimension-change card sort task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40(4), 448-456. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0886-1
Eaves, L. C., & Ho, H. H. (2008). Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38(4), 739-747. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0441-x
Eckerman, C. O., Davis, C. C., & Didow, S. M. (1989). Toddlers' emerging ways of achieving social coordinations with a peer. Child Development, 60(2), 440-453.
Feldman, R., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1997). Affect regulation and synchronicy in mother-infant play as precursors to the development of symbolic competence. . Infant Mental Health Journal, 18, 4-23.
Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (2002). Cognitive development (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P., Constantino, J., Findling, R. L., Hardan, A. Y., & Eng, C. (2012). Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(1), 28-40.e23. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.09.021
Gernsbacher, M. A. (2006). Toward a behavior of reciprocity. Journal of Developmental Processes, 1, 139-152.
Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(2), 74-82. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006
Gillberg, C., & Wing, L. (1999). Autism: not an extremely rare disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 99(6), 399-406.
Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(4), 404-411.
Hadwin, J. A., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1996). Can we teach children with autism to understand emotions, belief, or pretense. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 345-365.
Hammerstein, P. (2003). "Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? a protestant appeal,”. Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation, 83-94.
Happaney, K., Zelazo, P. D., & Stuss, D. T. (2004). Development of orbitofrontal function: current themes and future directions. Brain Cognition, 55(1), 1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.01.001
Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 26-32.
Howes, C. (1988). Peer interaction of young children. . Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,, 53, 1-78.
Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 212-229.
Hughes, C., Adlam, A., Happe, F., Jackson, J., Taylor, A., & Caspi, A. (2000). Good test--retest reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range of abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4), 483-490.
Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 477-492.
Hume, K., Loftin, R., & Lantz, J. (2009). Increasing independence in autism spectrum disorders: a review of three focused interventions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39(9), 1329-1338. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0751-2
Hutchins, T. L., Bonazinga, L. A., Prelock, P. A., & Taylor, R. S. (2008). Beyond false beliefs: the development and psychometric evaluation of the perceptions of children's theory of mind measure-experimental version (PCToMM-E). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38(1), 143-155. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0377-1
Hutchins, T. L., Prelock, P. A., & Bonazinga, L. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of the Theory of Mind Inventory (ToMI): a study of typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(3), 327-341. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1244-7
Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004a). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology 16(1), 137-155.
Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004b). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 16(1), 137-155.
Kasari, C., Locke, J., Gulsrud, A., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2011). Social networks and friendships at school: comparing children with and without ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 533-544. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1076-x
Kavanaugh, R. D. (2006). Pretend play and theory of mind. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issue (2nd ed., pp. 153-166). New York: Psychology Press
Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of "hot" executive function: the children's gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148-157
Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(7), 831-846.
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). Autism. Lancet, 383(9920), 896-910. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1
Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: a continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 557-573. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1
Leekam, S. R., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 261-273.
Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Mandy, W. P., Charman, T., & Skuse, D. H. (2012). Testing the construct validity of proposed criteria for DSM-5 autism spectrum disorder. Journal of American Academy in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(1), 41-50. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.10.013
Mastrangelo, S. (2009). Play and the child with autism spectrum disorder: from possibilities to practice. International Journal of Play Therapy, 18(1), 13-30.
Matson, J. L., Hess, J. A., Mahan, S., & Fodstad, J. C. (2010). Convergent validity of the Autism Spectrum Disorder Diagnostic for Children (ASD-DC) and Autism Diagnostic Interview-revised (ADI-R). Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 633-638
McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(4), 563-578.
McPartland, J. C., Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2012). Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of American Academy in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(4), 368-383. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.007
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3-19.
Montgomery, J. M., Stoesz, B., & McCrimmon, A. (2012). Emotional intelligence, theory of mind, and executive functions as predictors of social outcomes in young adults with Asperger Syndrome. . Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 28, 4-13. doi: 10.1177/1088357612461525.
O'Brien, S. K. (1996). The validity and reliability of the Wing Subgroups Questionnaire. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(3), 321-335.
Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: a new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 25(4), 415-433.
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51-87.
Perez, S. M., & Gauvain, M. (2005). The role of child emotionality in child behaviour and maternal instruction on planning taks. Social Development, 14, 250-272.
Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60(3), 688-700.
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). "John thinks that Mary thinks that..." attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39(3), 437-471.
Pillow, B. H., & Flavell, J. H. (1986). Young children's knowledge about visual perception: projective size and shape. Child Development 57(1), 125-135.
Portney, L. G., & Watkins, M. P. (2007). Foundations of clinical research applications to practice, Third Edition: Prentice Hall.
Premacka, D. a. W., G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526.
Prior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. L. (1990). Autistic children's knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(4), 587-601.
Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cognition, 55(1), 11-29. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00277-x
Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D., & McGrath, J. (1994). Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. Journal of Neurology,Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57(12), 1518-1524.
Rotheram-Fuller, E., Kasari, C., Chamberlain, B., & Locke, J. (2010). Social involvement of children with autism spectrum disorders in elementary school classrooms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(11), 1227-1234. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02289.x
Sakaiya, S., Shiraito, Y., Kato, J., Ide, H., Okada, K., Takano, K., & Kansaku, K. (2013). Neural correlate of human reciprocity in social interactions. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 239. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00239
Sally, D., & Hill, E. (2006). The development of interpersonal strategy: autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(1), 73-97. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2005.06.015
Scheeren, A. M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2012). Social interaction style of children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42(10), 2046-2055. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1451-x
Schieve, L. A., Rice, C., Devine, O., Maenner, M. J., Lee, L. C., Fitgerald, R., Wingate, M. S., Schendel, D., Pettygrove, S., van Naarden Braun, K., & Durkin, M. (2011). Have secular changes in perinatal risk factors contributed to the recent autism prevalence increase? development and application of a mathematical assessment model. Annals of epidemiology, 12, 930-945
Schieve, L. A., Rice, C., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Boyle, C. A., Kogan, M. D., Drews, C., & Devine, O. (2012). Parent-reported prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in US-born children: an assessment of changes within birth cohorts from the 2003 to the 2007 National Survey of Children's Health. Maternernal and Child Health Journal, 16 Suppl 1, S151-157. doi: 10.1007/s10995-012-1004-0
Schopler E, Reichler R & DeVellis R. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (10), 91–103
Schopler E, Reichler R, & Rochen Renner B. (1988). The childhood autism rating scale. Western Psychological Services
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921-929. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f
Solomon, M., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Anders, T. F. (2004). A social adjustment enhancement intervention for high functioning autism, Asperger's syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder NOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6), 649-668.
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D., A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Spence, S. J., Sharifi, P., & Wiznitzer, M. (2004). Autism spectrum disorder: screening, diagnosis, and medical evaluation. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 11(3), 186-195.
Stevens, J. R., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Why be nice? psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(2), 60-65. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.003
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 643-662.
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R., Bond, S., & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 213-225.
Tager–Flusberg, H. (2001). A re-examination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.). The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum., 173-193.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. The Behavioral Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675-691; discussion 691-735. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05000129
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 3-48.
van Ommeren, T. B., Begeer, S., Scheeren, A. M., & Koot, H. M. (2012). Measuring reciprocity in high functioning children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1001-1010. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1331-9
Walker, D. R., Thompson, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., Goldberg, J., Bryson, S. E., Mahoney, W. J., & Szatmari, P. (2004). Specifying PDD-NOS: a comparison of PDD-NOS, Asperger syndrome, and autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(2), 172-180. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200402000-00012
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301-1303. doi: 10.1126/science.1121448
Wellman H.M., & Banerjee M. (1991). Mind and emotion: children’s understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.(9),191–214.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & J., W. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128.
World Health Organization. (2001). International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization
Wu, W. T., Chang, C. F., Lu, T. H., & Chiu, S. C. (2004). Vineland adaptive behavior scales: Chinese version manual. (Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V.) Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological (Original work published 1984)
Yerys, B. E., Hepburn, S. L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). Executive function in preschoolers with autism: evidence consistent with a secondary deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1068-1079. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0250-7
Young, R. L., & Rodi, M. L. (2014). Redefining autism spectrum disorder using DSM-5: the implications of the proposed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(4), 758-765. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1927-3
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297-301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354-360.
Zelazo, P. D., & Mu¨ller, U. (2002). Executive functions in typical and atypical development. Handbook of childhood cognitive development 445-469.
Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: relations in early development. Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind, 71-93.
論文全文使用權限
  • 同意授權校內瀏覽/列印電子全文服務,於2021-02-19起公開。
  • 同意授權校外瀏覽/列印電子全文服務,於2021-02-19起公開。


  • 如您有疑問,請聯絡圖書館
    聯絡電話:(06)2757575#65773
    聯絡E-mail:etds@email.ncku.edu.tw